Press: When push comes to shove, what counts as a fight?

Mike Miller put together a nice write up of the lab’s recently-published paper for UC News. You can read the article here (Thanks Mike!!)

The paper was published in Current Zoology and is available (open access) here.

 


When push comes to shove, what is a fight?

UC creates effective way to decide whether to lump together similar animal behaviors

 

Biologists often study animal sociality by collecting observations about several types of behavioral interactions. These interactions can be things like severe fights, minor fights, cooperative food sharing, or grooming each other.

But to analyze animal behavior, researchers need to make decisions about how to categorize these interactions and how to code these behaviors during data collection. Turns out, this question can be complicated.

Researchers at the University of Cincinnati dug into this tricky question while studying monk parakeets. In new research, published in the journal Current Zoology, the team asked: How do you properly categorize two seemingly similar behaviors? The study was led by UC postdoctoral researcher Annemarie van der Marel who worked with UC doctoral students Sanjay Prasher, Claire O’Connell, and Chelsea Carminito and UC Assistant Professor Elizabeth Hobson.

“Biologists have to deal with this question: are the behaviors that we categorize as unique actually perceived as unique by the animals?” Hobson said. “How do the animals classify those behaviors?”

Four people in face masks stand in a socially distanced group.

UC biologists found a scientific way to decide whether similar animal behaviors should be grouped together or separated for research studies. Pictured from left are UC doctoral student Sanjay Prasher, UC postdoctoral researcher and study lead author Annemarie van der Marel, UC Assistant Professor Elizabeth Hobson and UC doctoral student Claire O’Connell. Not pictured are study co-authors Chelsea Carminito, Alexa Phillips and Bryan Kluever. Photo/Andrew Higley/UC Creative + Brand

Biases can easily lead researchers astray in making these judgement calls. “If you look at chimpanzees, smiling is an aggressive behavior. But in humans, smiling is friendly,” said van der Marel. “The behaviors look similar to us but the nuances of how the behaviors are used are very different.”

A portrait of Annemarie van der Marel

UC’s Annemarie van der Marel. Photo/Andrew Higley/UC Creative + Brand

 

Monk parakeets are highly social parrots that live in large colonies where they often interact with other individuals. The parakeets spend a large proportion of the day fighting, especially top-ranked birds, which makes it easy to distinguish them from birds ranked lower in the dominance hierarchy, said O’Connell. The birds demonstrate aggression in many ways. They bite, of course. But monk parakeets also like to take another parrot’s perch through menace or sheer force.

UC biologists observed two types of this “king of the hill” behavior: “displacements”, where one bird lunges at another bird and can bite to force that bird away, and “crowding”, where a threatened bird moves before an aggressor is within biting range. The team coded these behaviors as distinct because they appeared to differ in the level of aggression, with displacements appearing as a more severe form of aggression with a higher potential for injury. The parakeets also used these behaviors somewhat differently, with displacements occurring much more often than the crowding.

Annemarie van der Marel looks through bincoulars with a tablet on her lap and a spotting scope mounted on a tripod in front of her.

UC postdoctoral researcher Annemarie van der Marel observes monk parakeets with binoculars in a bird blind. Photo/Provided

 

The question is: were these behaviors essentially used in the same ways by the birds, so that they could be treated as the same types of events in later analyses? Pooling two behaviors has major research benefits such as creating a richer dataset for future analyses. But pooling also carries a risk — by generalizing, scientists could be losing nuances of behaviors that convey important information when considered alone, Hobson said.

To get to the bottom of this question, the team turned to computational analysis. They created a new computer model that compared the real aggression patterns to ones that were randomized. This approach allowed the team to test whether treating the two behaviors as interchangeable caused any changes in the social structure. “On top of being a great lab bonding experience, it was an exciting opportunity to learn how to use simulations to help answer research questions” said Prasher.

In the UC study, their computational analysis supported pooling the two behaviors. For the UC team, these results will help them plan their future analyses as they work to understand the complex social lives of these parakeets. More broadly, van der Marel said the new framework can also help other animal-behavior researchers make informed and data-driven decisions about when to pool together behaviors and when to separate them.

Featured image at top: Monk parakeets. Photo/Michael Miller

Four people stand spaced apart in face masks and winter coats on a grid of multicolored stone tiles.

UC Assistant Professor Elizabeth Hobson, left, is studying the social behavior of monk parakeets in her biology lab. Also pictured from left are UC postdoctoral research Annemarie van der Marel and UC doctoral students Sanjay Prasher and Claire O’Connell. Photo/Andrew Higley/UC Creative + Brand